Buy new:
-48% $9.89$9.89
Ships from: Amazon.com Sold by: Amazon.com
Save with Used - Good
$7.91$7.91
Ships from: Amazon Sold by: 2nd Life Aloha

Download the free Kindle app and start reading Kindle books instantly on your smartphone, tablet, or computer - no Kindle device required.
Read instantly on your browser with Kindle for Web.
Using your mobile phone camera - scan the code below and download the Kindle app.
Follow the authors
OK
Proust and the Squid: The Story and Science of the Reading Brain Paperback – August 26, 2008
Purchase options and add-ons
“Wolf restores our awe of the human brain—its adaptability, its creativity, and its ability to connect with other minds through a procession of silly squiggles.” — San Francisco Chronicle
How do people learn to read and write—and how has the development of these skills transformed the brain and the world itself ? Neuropsychologist and child development expert Maryann Wolf answers these questions in this ambitious and provocative book that chronicles the remarkable journey of written language not only throughout our evolution but also over the course of a single child’s life, showing why a growing percentage have difficulty mastering these abilities.
With fascinating down-to-earth examples and lively personal anecdotes, Wolf asserts that the brain that examined the tiny clay tablets of the Sumerians is a very different brain from the one that is immersed in today’s technology-driven literacy, in which visual images on the screen are paving the way for a reduced need for written language—with potentially profound consequences for our future.
- Print length336 pages
- LanguageEnglish
- PublisherHarper Perennial
- Publication dateAugust 26, 2008
- Dimensions5.31 x 0.76 x 8 inches
- ISBN-109780060933845
- ISBN-13978-0060933845
Book recommendations, author interviews, editors' picks, and more. Read it now
Frequently bought together

Customers who viewed this item also viewed
- Children with a rich repertoire of words and their associations will experience any text or any conversation in ways that are substantively different from children who do not have the same stored words and concepts.Highlighted by 527 Kindle readers
- Proust saw reading as a kind of intellectual “sanctuary,” where human beings have access to thousands of different realities they might never encounter or understand otherwise.Highlighted by 517 Kindle readers
- Learning to read begins the first time an infant is held and read a story. How often this happens, or fails to happen, in the first five years of childhood turns out to be one of the best predictors of later reading.Highlighted by 469 Kindle readers
Editorial Reviews
Review
“For everyone who has wondered how reading and writing happen, here is an entertaining, comprehensive, delightfully clear account of how our brain allowed us to become word magicians. A splendid achievement!” — Alberto Manguel, author of A History of Reading
“Wolf’s intriguing combination of linguistic history, sociology, psychology, and neuroscience is engaging and clear. The figures and illustrations as well as the wonderful literary quotes enrich her readable prose...Recommended.” — Library Journal
“[Maryanne Wolf] displays extraordinary passion and perceptiveness concerning the reading brain, its miraculous achievements and tragic dysfunctions.” — BookForum
“Everything Wolf says makes sense....She clearly knows her stuff.” — Washington Post Book World
“Wolf restores our awe of the human brain its adaptability, its creativity and its ability to connect with other minds through a procession of silly squiggles.” — San Francisco Chronicle
“Brilliant and eye-opening.” — Philadelphia Inquirer
“[Proust and the Squid] rises from a merely professional tome to a personal and highly accessible project.” — California Literary Review
“Brilliant and eye-opening.” — Albany Times Union
“Wolf’s knowledge of and appreciation for her subject are apparent....fascinating....Wolf restores our awe of the human brain its adaptability, its creativity and its ability to connect with other minds through a procession of silly squiggles.” — Lisa Ko, author of The Leavers
“[Wolf’s] conversational style, reflective comments and insights from work with children...create a narrative flow and bright tone.” — Minneapolis Star Tribune
“The squid of Wolf’s title represents the neurobiological approach to the study of reading....Given the panic that takes hold of humanists when the decline of reading is discussed, her cold-blooded perspective is opportune.” — The New Yorker
“A book worth talking about.” — U.S. News & World Report
“Enjoyable....Wolf, with remarkable agility in a relatively compact book (intended for both aficionados and the uninitiated), transitions seamlessly between disciplines as diverse as linguistics, neuroscience, cognitive psychology, and archeology, among others. Her voice comes through clearly; she is fascinated by reading and shares that energy.” — New England Journal of Medicine
“Wolf’s alarm about the spread of semi- literacy among the young is obviously justified, and her book provokes thought about it as only reading can.” — Sunday Times (London)
“This humane and fascinating book...is a paean to what Proust, über-reader, called ‘that fruitful miracle of a communication in the midst of solitude,’ to all that has been and can be achieved for individuals and for mankind through literacy.” — The Evening Standard (London)
“Blindingly fascinating...detailed and scholarly....There’s a lot of difficult material in here. But it’s worth the effort....For people interested in language, this is a must. You’ll find yourself focusing on words in new ways. Read it slowly--it will take time to sink in.” — The Sunday Telegraph
“Proust and the Squid is an inspiring celebration of the science of reading....Wolf’s insights are fascinating....Proust and the Squid has much to offer on this important--perhaps the most important--subject” — The Guardian (London)
“Her book is a remarkable excavation of something we take largely for granted, and throws up plenty of thought-provoking ideas along the way.” — Sunday Times (London)
From the Back Cover
"Human beings were never born to read," writes Tufts University cognitive neuroscientist and child development expert Maryanne Wolf. Reading is a human invention that reflects how the brain rearranges itself to learn something new. In this ambitious, provocative book, Wolf chronicles the remarkable journey of the reading brain not only over the past five thousand years, since writing began, but also over the course of a single child's life, showing in the process why children with dyslexia have reading difficulties and singular gifts.
Lively, erudite, and rich with examples, Proust and the Squid asserts that the brain that examined the tiny clay tablets of the Sumerians was a very different brain from the one that is immersed in today's technology-driven literacy. The potential transformations in this changed reading brain, Wolf argues, have profound implications for every child and for the intellectual development of our species.
About the Author
Maryanne Wolf, the John DiBiaggio Professor of Citizenship and Public Service at Tufts University, was the director of the Tufts Center for Reading and Language Research. She currently directs the Center for Dyslexia, Diverse Learners, and Social Justice at UCLA, and is working with the Dyslexia Center at the UCSF School of Medicine and with Curious Learning: A Global Literacy Project, which she co-founded. She is the recipient of multiple research and teaching honors, including the highest awards by the International Dyslexia Association and the Australian Learning Disabilities Association. She is the author of Proust and the Squid (HarperCollins), Tales of Literacy for the 21st Century (Oxford University Press), and more than 160 scientific publications.
Excerpt. © Reprinted by permission. All rights reserved.
Proust and the Squid
The Story and Science of the Reading BrainBy Maryanne WolfHarperCollins Publishers, Inc.
Copyright © 2008 Maryanne WolfAll right reserved.
ISBN: 9780060933845
Chapter One
Reading Lessons From Proust and the Squid
I believe that reading, in its original essence, [is] that fruitful miracle of a communication in the midst of solitude.
—Marcel Proust
Learning involves the nurturing of nature.
—Joseph LeDoux
We were never born to read. Human beings invented reading only a few thousand years ago. And with this invention, we rearranged the very organization of our brain, which in turn expanded the ways we were able to think, which altered the intellectual evolution of our species. Reading is one of the single most remarkable inventions in history; the ability to record history is one of its consequences. Our ancestors' invention could come about only because of the human brain's extraordinary ability to make new connections among its existing structures, a process made possible by the brain's ability to be shaped by experience. This plasticity at the heart of the brain's design forms the basis for much of who we are, and who we might become.
This book tells the story of the reading brain, in the context of our unfolding intellectual evolution. That story is changing before our eyes and under the tips of our fingers. The next few decades will witness transformations in our ability to communicate, as we recruit new connections in the brain that will propel our intellectual development in new and different ways. Knowing what reading demands of our brain and knowing how it contributes to our capacity to think, to feel, to infer, and to understand other human beings is especially important today as we make the transition from a reading brain to an increasingly digital one. By coming to understand how reading evolved historically, how it is acquired by a child, and how it restructured its biological underpinnings in the brain, we can shed new light on our wondrous complexity as a literate species. This places in sharp relief what may happen next in the evolution of human intelligence, and the choices we might face in shaping that future.
This book consists of three areas of knowledge: the early history of how our species learned to read, from the time of the Sumerians to Socrates; the developmental life cycle of humans as they learn to read in ever more sophisticated ways over time; and the story and science of what happens when the brain can't learn to read. Taken together, this cumulative knowledge about reading both celebrates the vastness of our accomplishment as the species that reads, records, and goes beyond what went before, and directs our attention to what is important to preserve.
There is something less obvious that this historical and evolutionary view of the reading brain gives us. It provides a very old and very new approach to how we teach the most essential aspects of the reading process—both for those whose brains are poised to acquire it and for those whose brains have systems that may be organized differently, as in the reading disability known as dyslexia. Understanding these unique hardwired systems—which are preprogrammed generation after generation by instructions from our genes—advances our knowledge in unexpected ways that have implications we are only beginning to explore.
Interwoven through the book's three parts is a particular view of how the brain learns anything new. There are few more powerful mirrors of the human brain's astonishing ability to rearrange itself to learn a new intellectual function than the act of reading. Underlying the brain's ability to learn reading lies its protean capacity to make new connections among structures and circuits originally devoted to other more basic brain processes that have enjoyed a longer existence in human evolution, such as vision and spoken language. We now know that groups of neurons create new connections and pathways among themselves every time we acquire a new skill. Computer scientists use the term "open architecture" to describe a system that is versatile enough to change—or rearrange—to accommodate the varying demands on it. Within the constraints of our genetic legacy, our brain presents a beautiful example of open architecture. Thanks to this design, we come into the world programmed with the capacity to change what is given to us by nature, so that we can go beyond it. We are, it would seem from the start, genetically poised for breakthroughs.
Thus the reading brain is part of highly successful two-way dynamics. Reading can be learned only because of the brain's plastic design, and when reading takes place, that individual brain is forever changed, both physiologically and intellectually. For example, at the neuronal level, a person who learns to read in Chinese uses a very particular set of neuronal connections that differ in significant ways from the pathways used in reading English. When Chinese readers first try to read in English, their brains attempt to use Chinese-based neuronal pathways. The act of learning to read Chinese characters has literally shaped the Chinese reading brain. Similarly, much of how we think and what we think about is based on insights and associations generated from what we read. As the author Joseph Epstein put it, "A biography of any literary person ought to deal at length with what he read and when, for in some sense, we are what we read."
These two dimensions of the reading brain's development and evolution—the personal-intellectual and the biological—are rarely described together, but there are critical and wonderful lessons to be discovered in doing just that. In this book I use the celebrated French novelist Marcel Proust as metaphor and the largely underappreciated squid as analogy for two very different aspects of reading. Proust saw reading as a kind of intellectual "sanctuary," where human beings have access to thousands of different realities they might never encounter or understand otherwise. Each of these new realities is capable of transforming readers' intellectual lives without ever requiring them to leave the comfort of their armchairs.
Scientists in the 1950s used the long central axon of the shy but cunning squid to . . .
Continues...
Excerpted from Proust and the Squidby Maryanne Wolf Copyright © 2008 by Maryanne Wolf. Excerpted by permission.
All rights reserved. No part of this excerpt may be reproduced or reprinted without permission in writing from the publisher.
Excerpts are provided by Dial-A-Book Inc. solely for the personal use of visitors to this web site.
Product details
- ASIN : 0060933844
- Publisher : Harper Perennial
- Publication date : August 26, 2008
- Edition : Reprint
- Language : English
- Print length : 336 pages
- ISBN-10 : 9780060933845
- ISBN-13 : 978-0060933845
- Item Weight : 2.31 pounds
- Dimensions : 5.31 x 0.76 x 8 inches
- Best Sellers Rank: #26,370 in Books (See Top 100 in Books)
- #19 in Linguistics Reference
- #21 in Reading Skills Reference (Books)
- #98 in Cognitive Psychology (Books)
- Customer Reviews:
About the authors
Discover more of the author’s books, see similar authors, read book recommendations and more.
Discover more of the author’s books, see similar authors, read book recommendations and more.
Customer reviews
- 5 star4 star3 star2 star1 star5 star76%19%5%0%0%76%
- 5 star4 star3 star2 star1 star4 star76%19%5%0%0%19%
- 5 star4 star3 star2 star1 star3 star76%19%5%0%0%5%
- 5 star4 star3 star2 star1 star2 star76%19%5%0%0%0%
- 5 star4 star3 star2 star1 star1 star76%19%5%0%0%0%
Customer Reviews, including Product Star Ratings help customers to learn more about the product and decide whether it is the right product for them.
To calculate the overall star rating and percentage breakdown by star, we don’t use a simple average. Instead, our system considers things like how recent a review is and if the reviewer bought the item on Amazon. It also analyzed reviews to verify trustworthiness.
Learn more how customers reviews work on AmazonCustomers say
Customers find the book informative about the science of reading and its history, with one noting how it connects readers to further resources. Moreover, the book receives positive feedback for its readability and information quality, with one customer highlighting how it presents complex ideas with clarity. Additionally, customers appreciate the book's creativity and find it a great listen.
AI Generated from the text of customer reviews
Select to learn more
Customers find the book informative and insightful, providing detailed information about the brain's reading process and the history of reading.
"...She has a lovely little call to action about how we can diagnose and treat all reading disorders and it's only a matter of access that is holding us..." Read more
"...Everything about this book is interesting, especially to those who may be described as "obsessive" readers that spend a great majority of their life..." Read more
"...Although Wolf's book presents a large amount of research, its engaging narrative and prevalent use of anecdotes keeps the information accessible to..." Read more
"A great look at how the brain learns to read with tips for helping learners." Read more
Customers find the book enjoyable to read and consider it vital reading, with one customer noting it provides a great history of reading.
"...not reading this will give you a nice pat on the back and a great history of reading and our understanding of how the mind does what it does...." Read more
"...of reading repurposes the neural networks we were born with in a great feat of plasticity and stresses that we as a species were never designed to..." Read more
"Maryanne Wolf has written a deeply rewarding exploration of reading and its impact on the human brain -- from a historical perspective, from a..." Read more
"...ESL teacher, what I learned in this book was very insightful and a great resource for helping me better understand the students with whom I work." Read more
Customers appreciate the book's information quality, noting its excellent overview and abundant examples, with one customer highlighting how it integrates technical brain science with clarity.
"...neuroscience of reading, the author gives the reader an excellent overview of the cultural origins of writing/reading, the brain mechanisms that are..." Read more
"...narrative and prevalent use of anecdotes keeps the information accessible to most readers; however to fully enjoy the book, it is helpful for..." Read more
"...human cultures and to its ability to give humans " access to thousands of different realities they might never encounter or understand otherwise."..." Read more
"Hard to follow through the book. There are some great facts and analogies in the book to learn about and understand how we learn to read and how..." Read more
Customers appreciate the creativity of the book, with one describing it as a beautiful and in-depth look at the subject matter.
"...disorders and it's only a matter of access that is holding us back, beautiful and idealistic but if you've ever been in a poor community and homes..." Read more
"...are often thought to be slow learners, but often they are bright and creative...." Read more
"This book gave an in depth look at how our brains learn to read...." Read more
"Maryanne Wolf.. Sigh. What a beautiful and accessible writer and neuroscientist you are. Please write more books." Read more
Customers find the book to be a great listen.
"...I got the book used and also the audio version. Great listen!" Read more
"It is perhaps more enjoyable to listen to this on the CDs, as I did while waiting for the paperback to come out...." Read more
"I bought this for my husband. He enjoyed it very much. He said this is not a quick read but he recommends it." Read more
Reviews with images

Learning to read is critical
Top reviews from the United States
There was a problem filtering reviews. Please reload the page.
- Reviewed in the United States on March 14, 2024Format: PaperbackVerified PurchaseIn a sentence: Reading changes our mind and in doing so changes our reading in a compounding feedback loop that has literally rewired the human mind.
Who should read it: Everyone, it's super interesting and if you're an avid reader who knows that reading is better than not reading this will give you a nice pat on the back and a great history of reading and our understanding of how the mind does what it does. If you love being pretentious this is a good book too, the author is full of it haha. Did you know Socrates was avidly against literacy and it's harm to the mind? I knew he was an annoying ass who his neighbors voted to have killed but didn't know about his objections the dangers reading had on wisdome.
Proust: Author who captured the profound impact reading has on someone's life
Squid: An old neurological metaphor for how neurons work.
The book outlines the literal rewiring necessary for humans to learn how to read and how profound and different that is than other built in attributes our minds have.
'this plasticity at the heart of the brain's design forms the basis for much of who we are, and who we might become'
Throughout the book I'm struck by wondering how much more we could be doing to rewire our minds intentionally through systems like reading. How much can we do? To change our "open architecture" of the brain. What are we leaving on the table? And how much are we harming the kids who are being left out of this rewiring. Much like everything else we do this feedback loop seems to be part of the Matthew principle and the author talks much about this. The rich get rich the poor get poorer.
"we are, it would seem from the start, genetically poised for breakthroughs"
"thus the reading brain is part of highly successful two way dynamics. Reading can be learned only because of the brains plastic design, and when reading takes place, that individual brain is forever changed, both physiologically and intellectually."
'we are what we read'
The moment we become literate 'we are no longer limited by the confines of our own thinking'
'the richness of this semantic dimension of reading depends on the riches we have already stored, a fact with important and sometimes devastating developmental implications for our children. Children with a rich repertoire of words and their associations will experience any text or any conversation win ways that are substantively different from children who do not have the same stored words and concepts'
'we bring our entire store of meanings to whatever we read - or not'
'If there are no genes specific only to reading, and if our brain has to connect older structures for vision and language to learn this new skill, every child in every generation has a lot of work'
'owing largely to their environments, however, one child will acquire these essentials, the other will not'
'learning to read begins the first time an infant is held and read a story. How often this happens, or fails to happen, in the first years of childhood turns out to be one of the best predictors of later reading'
'every child who learns to read someone else's thoughts and write his or her own repeats this cyclical, germinating relationship between written language and new thought, never before imagined'
'vygotsky observed that the very process of writing one's thoughts leads individuals to refine those thoughts and to discover new ways of thinking.' (does interacting with your personal LLM create a hyper feedback loop of this? Are we on the cusp of something new)
'the association between hearing written language and feeling loved provides the best foundation for this long process, and no cognitive scientist or educational researcher could have designed a better one'
Reading is essential to learning to understand someone else's mind, understanding someone else's life, feelings, experiences. Otherwise known as empathy (unless you're only reading the quran)
She has a lovely little call to action about how we can diagnose and treat all reading disorders and it's only a matter of access that is holding us back, beautiful and idealistic but if you've ever been in a poor community and homes you know access ain't the problem brother. She says 'a level playing field for all children before they enter kindergarten should not be that difficult to achieve' the understatement of the millennium .
Note: Look up Katie Overy, Catherine Moritz, Sasha Yampolsky research into Early Intervention based on rhythm, melody, and rhyme
Decoding is necessary to reading but 'one of the biggest errors in reading instruction is the assumption that after Amelia finally decodes a word she knows what she is reading' 'decoding does NOT mean comprehension'
Nightmare Fuel: 30 to 40 percent of children in the fourth grade do not become fully fluent readers with adequate comprehension
The two greatest aids to fluent comprehension are explicit instruction by a child's teachers in major content areas and the child's own desire to read
'having a richly connected, established vocabulary or semantic network is physically reflected in the brain'
'reading changes our lives an dour lives change our reading' its bidirectional
The author spends a great deal of time on dyslexia, it's a mystery basically, it can be wiring, structural, connective, any number of things.
'rapid automatized naming' (RAN) tasks are one of the best predictors of reading performance
' some young children with severe reading disabilities come from such linguistically impoverished backgrounds that vocabulary plays a critical role' not shit, reading isn't genetic, its an environmental problem in almost all but the most severe disabilities. If we know how to treat nearly all cases of learning disabilities it means environment (parents and caretakers) are to blame. Stop protecting bad parents
It is the oral word that first illuminates consciousness = Language IS cognition
Technology: Their sights are narrowed to what they see and hear quickly and easily, and they have too little reason to think outside our newest, most sophisticated boxes. These students are not illiterate, but they may never become true expert readers
What does the future hold? A nation of semi literate people incapable of reasoning beyond the customized LLM text in front of them designed to get them to act a certain way?
- Reviewed in the United States on July 10, 2008Format: HardcoverVerified PurchaseIn this fascinating work, which might be viewed as an introduction to the cognitive neuroscience of reading, the author gives the reader an excellent overview of the cultural origins of writing/reading, the brain mechanisms that are responsible for the ability to read, and the factors behind the inability to read. Written for a general audience, the book does contain some information of a more technical nature for those readers who might have a general background in neuroscience or cognitive neuroscience. Those readers who need more can find much more detailed information in the references. Everything about this book is interesting, especially to those who may be described as "obsessive" readers that spend a great majority of their life reading and are interested in knowing more about the cognitive mechanisms behind the reading act.
There are many interesting discussions and questions that are provoked by the reading of this book. Some of these include:
- Once one has achieved what the author has called "expert" reading status, what is the effect of biological age on this status? Does biological aging affect the "rate of processing" of textual information and if so to what degree? Along these same lines, is it more difficult for an older person to learn how to read as compared to young children?
- Erotic literature has the propensity for physical arousal, so does its reading evoke even more of the imaginative properties of the reading brain than does other types of literature or less? In addition, it would seem that the limbic system would play a greater role in erotic literature, since more emphasis is being placed on attention and imagination than comprehension.
- The technical description that author gives of the "first 500 milliseconds" of reading is fascinating and sheds light on the degree to which the reader must be attentive to the words in the text. But in relation to the need for this attention, while reading a book everyone no doubt has experienced the process of "drifting": you are turning the pages of the book and reading the text but your mind is engaged in other thoughts far removed from the content of the book. After some time and possibly many pages later you catch yourself and then skim the pages you thought you missed. Is the information in the book still assimilated when "drifting" or is completely ignored because the reader is not exercising deliberate concentration? Or is it being partially assimilated and to what degree? And if only partially, can the "skimming" fill in the lost details? If one believes the author's technical description then when "drifting" certain areas of the `parietal lobe', those that are responsible for "disengaging" attention from whatever else we are doing, are not being activated, but the `superior colliculi' that is responsible for eye movements, and the `thalamus' that coordinates information from the brain are.
- Is "speed reading" a viable or effective strategy and what exactly is behind it? Does speed reading require other neuronal mechanisms over and above what is discussed in the "first 500 milliseconds"? People who claim to be able to speed read usually also claim that they do so with complete comprehension. Is this true or are they missing some important information from the book? Unfortunately the author does not discuss speed reading in this book.
- Does it become easier to assimilate knowledge the more one reads? If one accepts the author's explanations this would be the case, for she holds that less time is required for a "fluent" brain to represent and retrieve the visual, phonological, and semantic information needed for reading. But in this regard is it possible to read "too much", i.e. to read at such an intensity/frequency that a kind of "asymptotic limit" is reached for the ability to retrieve information from `associative' memory as described by the author?
- Is the reading process as discussed by the author different to some degree when reading technical literature? Those who read mathematical texts can attest to the large degree of concentration needed as compared for example to reading a novel or a news story. The author asserts that the speed that we read a word is influenced greatly by the quality and quantity of the semantic or background knowledge that is activated by that word. But does this also hold for mathematical equations or other types of symbolic expressions that are essentially outside colloquial grammars? English grammar for example does not include mathematical expressions as part of its syntax or semantics so when such expressions are included in texts, as they are of course in mathematical texts written in English, the reader's "flow" must be interrupted so as to deal with these expressions. This slows down the reading rate considerably, and frequently a lot of backtracking must be done in order to fully comprehend the text. Ironically, visualization plays a strong role in the understanding of mathematical texts, but the authors of these texts frequently eschew the idea of incorporating diagrams or pictures in them.
- The author devotes a considerable part of the book to the historical invention of language and reading and compares the skepticism of Socrates towards writing/reading to her own skepticism on the use of online tools for the presentation of information. As far as the explanatory power of verbal narratives are concerned, Socrates certainly had a point if one is only concerned with dialogs of a philosophical or argumentative nature, as of course Socrates was deeply embedded in. But think of how difficult it would be exchange highly sophisticated mathematical information in a verbal dialog. Such an exchange almost necessitates the use of writing, as well as its preservation. And as far as online information and the way it is presented, the jury is still out on its efficacy due to the short timeline that the Internet has been available to everyone. In this regard the author, and all of those who love to read, must be careful not to morph into technoreactionaries when dealing with the new methods of presenting information. These new methods may be even more effective, even more fun, than the activity we have all done for thousands of years, this activity which at some point in the future may be christened as "classical reading."
Top reviews from other countries
-
neunzertReviewed in Germany on June 16, 2017
5.0 out of 5 stars Sehr spannendes Buch mit originellem, aber schwer verständlichem Titel
Format: PaperbackVerified PurchaseDas ist ein sehr spannendes Buch, leider gibt es nichts vergleichbares in deutsch. Es kam ja antiquarisch aus USA, war aber gut erhalten und ist wirklich lesenswert. Der Titel ist ein schöner Hinweis auf den Inhalt, aber sicher nicht leicht verständlich. Das Buch hält aber, was der Titel verspricht.
-
MatildaReviewed in Spain on August 13, 2013
4.0 out of 5 stars Un acercamiento científico al logro de la lectura y al problema de la dislexia
Este libro presenta un panorama general sobre los procesos neurológicos que tienen lugar durante el aprendizaje de la lectura. Es bastante ameno, se aprende con él e invita a pensar. Se echa de menos un glosario porque a veces es muy difícil comprender los términos técnicos.
- -K-Reviewed in India on August 8, 2022
5.0 out of 5 stars Loved the book! Highly recommended...
Format: KindleVerified PurchaseI would recommend this book to anyone who's interested in learning more about reading and the science behind it...really good book
-
深川高山Reviewed in Japan on February 10, 2010
5.0 out of 5 stars 読みやすいですよ。
Format: PaperbackVerified Purchase邦訳版が読みづらかったので、原書を買ってみました。引用文以外は、読みやすい文章です。専門用語は、日本語でも意味がわかりませんから、気にせず読んでしまいましょう。引用文は文学的で、素人には少し荷が重いですね。
- Bryon L WhiteReviewed in Canada on March 30, 2025
5.0 out of 5 stars Terrible Title But Great Book
Format: KindleVerified PurchaseThe title of this book is horrible and doesn’t give readers any insight to the book’s contents. Proust put me to sleep, BUT this is a really interesting read about how writing evolved, how the brain reads and the connection to neurological development, and why some struggle. Unlike Proust or squids, it kept me turning pages.